In public political debate there are those who want to blame
the poor for their own poverty, perhaps characterizing them as lazy or lacking
some other kind of character trait a person needs to earn money. Recently,
celebrity cook Jamie Oliver generated controversy with a comment beginning with
a disclaimer which tends to set off the alarm bells, “I’m not judgmental, but…”
(uh-oh)
"I'm not judgmental, but I've spent a lot of time in poor communities, and I find it quite hard to talk about modern-day poverty. You might remember that scene in [a previous series] Ministry of Food, with the mum and the kid eating chips and cheese out of Styrofoam containers, and behind them is a massive fucking TV. It just didn't weigh up."The fascinating thing for me is that seven times out of 10, the poorest families in this country choose the most expensive way to hydrate and feed their families. The ready meals, the convenience foods." [1]
An article published in Science
last week [2] described an experiment where people were asked to do a simple
‘which-shape-fits-in-the-blank’ task similar to some questions which might
appear in IQ tests. They gave some good evidence that simply being poorer,
having financial issues on the mind, can make it more difficult to perform on
their test. It’s a nice demonstration that actually fits with the popular idea that in some way, poverty is a result of poor
people’s behavior, while underscoring that a person’s behavior is affected by
circumstance and being financially secure can make it easier to act smarter.
Conversely, being financially vulnerable makes good decision-making harder and
could potentially trap people into a cycle of poverty for which you can hardly
blame them. The effect of financial vulnerability could be pretty strong; the
experimenters compared the effects they found with other studies and suggested
that “evoking financial concerns has a cognitive impact comparable with losing
a full night of sleep”.
The
experimenters wanted to make sure that the gap in between the rich and poor
they saw didn’t just hold for the moment right after people face a tough
financial decision. To do that they found that sugar cane farmers from Tamil
Nadu in India struggled significantly more (p<0.001) with the
‘fill-the-blank’ tasks before harvest than after harvest, when they got paid
and had fewer financial difficulties. The effect held no matter what time of
year harvest occurred. Experimenters even collected heart rate and blood
pressure as a way of measuring stress, and they found that even taking those
factors into account, farmers still struggled more with the task before
harvest. That showed that the effect they saw wasn’t fully caused by stress, at
least as far as stress measured by heart rate and blood pressure is concerned.
Though stress could have been a factor, the researchers thought that because an
effect remained regardless of stress, the mere background cognitive demand on
the farmers during hard financial times caused them to perform more poorly.
Is this even a new
thing?
This study does a good job demonstrating a link between
financial circumstances and cognitive ability. But it is far from the first to
examine the nasty way poverty works as a catch-22 to trap people and stop them
from getting ahead in the way we’d like to think people can in an egalitarian
or meritocratic society. Childhood poverty seems to predict the size of
people’s working memory, and mainly through chronic stress, i.e., child poverty
is related to chronic stress and that chronic stress caused most of the
relationship between poverty and a poor working memory as an adult [3].
IQ is even correlated worldwide with national GDPs, and when GDP rises, so do
IQs, suggesting that access to money and resources helps determine IQ, not (or
not only) the other way around [4].
So…?
Based on what they found, the study authors suggested that
government work and income programs need to avoid adding stress and complexity
to application processes for receiving income support or assistance in finding
work. If beneficiaries are to be helped back into work, too much cognitive load
spent thinking and worrying just about access to benefits might hinder their
ability to find work. Government austerity measures which introduce new hoops
beneficiaries must jump through to access government support might be intended
to reduce the amount of money the government spends on income support. In the
long run, the cognitive load effect observed in the study could work against
such austerity measures, working to increase the amount of time it takes for
people to find work and hence be dependent on government support.
More broadly, the new Science
study shows that, where people in poverty are struggling to get ahead, poverty itself
can prevent them from leaving poverty behind, through a harmful, though
temporary, effect on intelligence. Thus, not only is it futile to blame this
detrimental effect on someone’s cognitive ability on them, since it is outside
of their control, the poverty effect could be relieved if only a person had relief from
poverty and excessive financial concerns. Such a fact has substantial
implications if we want to make our society any kind of egalitarian or
meritocratic one in which the skills of every individual are fully realized for
society as a whole.
[1] Deans,
J. Jamie Oliver bemoans chips, cheese and giant TVs of modern-day poverty. The Guardian, 26 August 2012. Retrieved
from http://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/2013/aug/27/jamie-oliver-chips-cheese-modern-day-poverty
on 31 August 2013.
[2] Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E., & Zhao, J. (2013) Poverty Impedes
Cognitive Function. Science 30 August
2013: 976-980. Retrieved from http://www.sciencemag.org/content/341/6149/976.on
on 31 August 2013.
[3] Evans, G. W. (2009) Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working
memory. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(16):6545-6549. Retrieved
from http://www.pnas.org/content/106/16/6545.long
on 31 August 2013.
[4] Roiviainen, E. (2012) Economic, educational, and IQ gains in eastern Germany
1990-2006. Intelligence 40(6):
571-575. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289612000864
on 31 August 2013.
No comments:
Post a Comment